Universidad Alberto Hurtado, Santiago, Chile
i.gonzalezpalta@gmail.com
Recibido: 09/Octumbre/2023
Revisado: 29/Abril/2024
Aprobado: 29/Junio/2024
Publicado: 25/Septiembre/2024
Argumentation and Knowledge Construction in Higher Education: A Vygotskian Perspective
Abstract
Empirical research indicates that the utilization of argumentation as an educational strategy fosters the acquisition of argumentation skills and disciplinary knowledge. Furthermore, there is evidence to show that argumentation skills development mediates the effect of oral argumentation on disciplinary knowledge. However, studies have also shown that argumentation is scarce in higher education and that although lecturers see argumentation as a relevant pedagogical goal, they think that students need to first acquire knowledge in order to develop argumentation skills. Therefore, a crucial pedagogical resource to promote scientific knowledge in higher education is being overlooked. This paper is aimed at shedding light onto argumentation as a key practice in higher education teaching, disputing the idea that to argue, students first need to acquire knowledge. We elaborate further on Vygotsky’s notion of scientific concepts, highlighting their dialectic structure, which accounts for the epistemic potential of argumentation. This paper extends an invitation to higher education lecturers to prompt argumentation in classrooms as a form of disciplinary learning. Limitations and implications are discussed throughout the paper.
Keywords: argumentation, scientific concepts, knowledge, higher education teaching
Para citar este artículo: González-Palta, I., & Larrain , A. (2024). Argumentación y Construcción de Conocimiento en Alumnos de Educación Superior: una Perspectiva Vygotskiana. Praxis & Saber, 15(41), 1–18.
Ingrid Noemí González-Palta 1
Antonia Larrain Sutil
Argumentación y Construcción de Conocimiento en Alumnos de Educación Superior: una Perspectiva Vygotskiana
Resumen
La investigación empírica indica que el uso de la argumentación como estrategia educativa fomenta la adquisición de habilidades de argumentación y conocimientos disciplinarios. Además, hay evidencia que muestra que el desarrollo de habilidades de argumentación ayuda a mejorar la argumentación oral de los alumnos en torno al conocimiento disciplinario. Sin embargo, varios estudios también han demostrado que la argumentación es escasa en la educación superior y que, aunque los profesores ven la argumentación como un objetivo pedagógico relevante, piensan que los estudiantes primero necesitan adquirir conocimientos para poder desarrollar habilidades argumentativas. Debido a esto, se está desaprovechando un recurso pedagógico crucial capaz de promover el conocimiento científico en la educación superior. Este artículo tiene como objetivo destacar la argumentación como una práctica clave en la enseñanza de la educación superior, disputando la idea de que, para argumentar, los estudiantes primero necesitan adquirir conocimientos. La investigación también profundiza en la perspectiva de Vygotsky sobre los conceptos científicos, subrayando su estructura dialéctica, la cual explica el potencial epistémico de la argumentación. Asimismo, este artículo extiende una invitación a los profesores de educación superior a fomentar la argumentación en las aulas como una forma de aprendizaje disciplinario. Las limitaciones e implicaciones se discuten a lo largo del artículo.
Palabras clave: argumentación, conceptos científicos, conocimiento, enseñanza en la educación superior
Argumentação e Construção do Conhecimento em Estudantes do Ensino Superior: uma Perspectiva Vygotskiana
Resumo
Pesquisas empíricas apontam que o uso da argumentação como estratégia pedagógica favorece tanto o desenvolvimento de habilidades argumentativas quanto a aquisição de conhecimento em diferentes áreas disciplinares. Além disso, há evidências de que essa prática melhora a argumentação oral dos estudantes ao tratar de temas científicos. Entretanto, diversas pesquisas revelam que a argumentação é pouco explorada no ensino superior. Embora os docentes reconheçam sua importância como objetivo pedagógico, muitos acreditam que os alunos devem primeiro dominar o conteúdo antes de desenvolverem suas competências argumentativas. Essa abordagem resulta na subutilização de um recurso pedagógico valioso, capaz de impulsionar a construção do conhecimento científico. Este artigo visa destacar a argumentação como prática essencial no ensino superior, desafiando a ideia de que o domínio prévio de conteúdos seja um requisito para o desenvolvimento da argumentação. A pesquisa também explora a perspectiva de Vygotsky sobre os conceitos científicos, enfatizando sua natureza dialética e o potencial epistêmico da argumentação. Ademais, o artigo convida os docentes incorporarem a argumentação em suas práticas de ensino como meio de promover a aprendizagem disciplinar. As limitações e implicações dessa abordagem são discutidas ao longo do texto.
Palavras-chave: argumentação, conceitos científicos, conhecimento, ensino superior
Introduction
The central task of university education is to train professionals, which is a major challenge. Part of this challenge is for students to reconstruct their own knowledge of their disciplines and professions in order to share their ways of thinking and to conceive the world in which we live from these perspectives. Obviously, achieving this goal is a complex matter, especially in the current reality of the highly overcrowded and diverse university system (Mok & Marginson, 2021).
The massification and diversification of the university system have led to greater access to higher education by the population and, although coverage is still insufficient (as students of low socioeconomic status have less opportunity to study in higher education) (Vandelannote & Demanet, 2021), there has been progress in this regard, particularly in countries where the state’s support and sources of financing have increased. This has generated a change in the entry profile of students by allowing access to a group of students that was previously excluded. In countries where the educational system is highly segregated, and the socioeconomic group of origin is intimately linked to the quality of the education to which they have access (Gutiérrez & Carrasco, 2021), this massification of the university system translates into very unequal entry characteristics, both in skills and previous knowledge.
Thus, although there is greater access to the university education system, the diversification of the entry profile has also evidenced a series of disadvantages with which these students deal, impacting on the permanence and effective term of the chosen career (Ives & Castillo-Montoya, 2020). Faced with this situation, the university has the formative challenge of ensuring that all students, regardless of their previous training, reconstruct the specialized knowledge of their disciplines and professions.
In this regard, the evidence indicates that argumentation constitutes a pedagogical practice especially powerful that enhances learning and the construction of knowledge (Driver et al., 2000; Jiménez-Aleixandre & Erduran, 2008). Specifically, empirical studies show that argumentative practice is crucial for the development of an argumentative ability, both at basic school levels (Bulgren et al., 2014) as well as at high education levels (Fan et al., 2020) as it promotes learning and knowledge acquisition (Iordanou et al., 2019). Furthermore, empirical studies indicate a correlation between the practice of argumentation, the enhancement of individual argumentative abilities, and the advancement of knowledge among school-aged children (Larrain et al., 2020; Vogel et al., 2017). This implies that these skills may serve as a mediating factor when using argumentation as a tool for the development of scientific understanding.
Nevertheless, two challenges persist. Firstly, while university students possess foundational argumentation skills (Larrain et al., 2015), the impact of university education on the enhancement of these skills is minimal, often reinforcing pre-existing disparities. Consequently, if argumentative skills, which are crucial for knowledge formation, remain underdeveloped during university education, it would lead to inequity among students in acquiring this essential cognitive tool for knowledge construction. Secondly, the absence of argumentative practice within the university setting, which is essential for fostering knowledge and skill development to bridge entry-level disparities, is notable (Archila et al., 2020). Additionally, a study in Chile revealed that educators in fields like Law and Philosophy view argumentation as a pedagogical tool primarily for skill enhancement rather than knowledge acquisition, mistakenly believing that disciplinary knowledge is necessary for teaching argumentation (Larrain et al., 2021). This perspective overlooks the potential of university education to cultivate both knowledge and critical skills, such as argumentation
The absence of reflection on argumentation and its role in knowledge construction within higher education literature, despite its recognition in Piaget’s work, may be attributed to a prevailing transversal conception within the academia. This conception often views argumentation more as a linguistic-cognitive process rather than a means of thinking and constructing knowledge1. This article aims to contribute to the discourse on teaching in higher education by exploring the epistemic nature of argumentation, which is crucial for fostering not only knowledge but also essential skills such as argumentative skills
Development of argumentative skills in Higher Education: A question of fairness
Research indicates that university students often exhibit limited argumentative abilities, particularly in written form (Larrain et al., 2015). Despite the recognized importance of argumentation in the learning process across various disciplines (Fan et al., 2020; Si et al., 2019), the university system frequently fails to teach argumentation explicitly or systematically (Larrain et al., 2021; Quintana & Correnti, 2019). This lack of emphasis on argumentation as a transversal skill may contribute to the underdevelopment of students’ argumentative skills, which are essential for constructing knowledge and engaging in critical thinking.
Although arguing is not taught, argumentation is a skill that is evaluated in higher education (Andrews et al., 2006), which ultimately impacts the student’s development. This is problematic because it implies that university assessment not only rests on the learning that is actually taught but on competencies and skills that are acquired and that are unequally distributed in students, contributing to the reproduction of existing social structures: students with greater cultural capital will be more qualified to deploy the argumentative language skills required in evaluative situations than those who have not previously developed this skill. In the current university system, facing this problem is an ethical duty associated with the principle of justice in higher education.
In this regard, we could assume that a logical way to address the problem would be to teach students to argue in order to develop the linguistic skills – diverse and unequal – that students bring as characteristics of entry into the university system. The problem we face is not only that opportunities for teaching argumentative skills are scarce, but that there is a debate about what space is most effective to develop this skill (intra or extracurricular) and how to do it (implicitly or explicitly). On the latter, there is some evidence that suggests that explicit teaching of argumentation would be more effective than implicit (Tiruneh et al., 2014). However, the explicit teaching of argumentation in courses or workshops specially designed for it, either within or complementary to the curriculum, despite allowing the promotion of general argumentation skills, does not promote the learning of the discursive genre typical of disciplinary argumentation (Larrain & Burrows, 2020). An alternative is to promote argumentation skills to learn the discipline through the sustained and transversal practice of argumentation as a means of disciplinary learning.
Argumentation in the classroom as a teaching and learning strategy of the disciplines
Argumentation, as a socio-discursive practice in which one or more speakers deal with a controversial issue through the formulation of arguments and counterarguments, articulating theory and evidence (Archila et al., 2020) to reach an understanding, has traditionally been conceived as closely linked to thought (Billig, 1996; Kuhn, 2010) and the construction of knowledge (Leitão, 2000; Schwarz, 2009). The epistemic nature of argumentation is indeed recognized for its role in enhancing scientific competencies such as reasoning, critical thinking, and scientific thinking (Hasnunidah et al., 2020; Sönmez et al., 2021; Osborne, 2010; Bulgren et al., 2014). In fact, research consistently shows that argumentative practice positively impacts disciplinary learning, helping students to better structure their reasoning, consider alternative viewpoints, and develop a deeper understanding of the subject matter (Nnanyereugo & Bolaji, 2020; Asterhan & Schwarz, 2016).
Research with schoolchildren supports this notion, indicating that peer-to-peer argumentation, particularly the articulation of counterarguments, fosters conceptual development (Larrain et al., 2019). Additionally, studies demonstrate that engaging in argumentative discourse within the classroom to acquire disciplinary content enhances both disciplinary learning and general argumentation skills in children (Iordanou et al., 2019). Furthermore, it is suggested that general argumentation skills serve as a mediator in the influence of argumentative practice on knowledge construction (Larrain et al., 2020).
Arguing in the classroom indirectly fosters knowledge by enhancing individual argumentation skills, which contribute to the social construction of knowledge. At the university level, research has shown that argumentative practice positively affects both argumentation skills and knowledge progression (Antonio & Prudente, 2021; Mulyati & Hadianto, 2022). However, these studies have not explored the mediating role of argumentation skills in knowledge construction, a phenomenon previously observed in schoolchildren. While the evidence on the mediating role of argumentation does not directly address university contexts, it is essential to consider potential differences that warrant investigation. The dual benefit of argumentation practice is significant, as it enhances the construction of disciplinary knowledge through the development of individual argumentation skills. These skills are cultivated through social interactions and argumentative experiences related to disciplinary content within the classroom setting.
Studies indicate that argumentative practice is notably lacking in the university setting, particularly within scientific education (Archila et al., 2020; Pabuccu & Erduran, 2017). It is observed that educators often favor traditional teaching methods, which typically involve minimal student interaction (Børte et al., 2020). Consequently, this approach may result in missed opportunities to enhance disciplinary knowledge among students, potentially exacerbating disparities in entry-level competencies. Furthermore, from an epistemological point of view, the absence of systematic argumentative practice in student education may lead to a perception of scientific and disciplinary knowledge as a mere accumulation of static and unquestionable facts (Driver et al., 2000). This perspective undermines the educational objective of preparing professionals who recognize the provisional nature of knowledge (Kuhn, 1962) and appreciate the significance of its critical assessment.
The conceptions of argumentation in university education
The question arises as to why argumentation, a recognized pedagogical strategy in school education, is not similarly emphasized in higher education discourse. A potential explanation is the nascent stage of pedagogical reflection in higher education, which grapples with the challenges of disciplinary diversity and a lack of professional training among lecturers for such strategies (Daniels, 2017; Daumiller et al., 2020; Fraser et al., 2019; Ndebele, 2022). Since argumentation still remains an underexplored and poorly communicated concept, it is not recognized as a learning discipline. Instead, the broader concept of active learning strategies serves as an umbrella term that includes various teaching methods, including argumentation.
Larrain et al. (2021) show that even in careers where argumentation should be a focus of reflection both as an objective and as a means of learning, the former is found but not the latter. For example, in the teaching of Philosophy and Law, the practice of argumentation is mainly conceived as a way to develop argumentation skills, recognized as relevant to professional practice, once students have the necessary knowledge. A conception of argumentation as a means of disciplinary learning is missed. These beliefs and conceptions would not only be the basis of the pedagogical choices of lecturers but also, above all, of curricular designs that would not visualize the potential of arguing in the classroom to build knowledge of the disciplines.
The classical theoretical explanations of argumentative discourse derived from the theory of argumentation have focused on its linguistic character (Domínguez & Conforti, 2019). Generally speaking, according to Buitrago et al. (2013), from scientific language and discursive approaches, argumentation can be understood as a competence or cognitive-linguistic skill derived from cognitive skills.
If argumentation is theorized as a linguistic product derived from cognitive skills, in that order (Buitrago et al., 2013), then it is understandable that, for example, extracurricular institutionalized spaces focus on the latter and not on teaching and giving opportunities to develop argumentation practice. The same argument is valid for teaching strategies: the focus is on developing in some way cognitive skills (such as understanding, applying, etc) among the students. While this happens, it is assumed that the student can express and communicate linguistically that knowledge mentally represented and developed from the lecturer’s discourse in the classroom.
If argumentation is thus distinguished and conceptualized as an argumentation product – without influence or relevant link with the thought process – even if such a distinction may be useful in analytical terms, it hides the processual and dialogical nature of argumentative practice (Larrain et al., 2019) and ignores the discursive and dialectical character of the construction of scientific knowledge. Moreover, it hinders the recognition of the interrelationship between these concepts and the transformative potential of argumentative practice for developing scientific knowledge.
In this way, this article aims to contribute to raising an epistemic notion of argumentation that highlights its socio-cultural nature and its intimate role in the construction of disciplinary knowledge. To do so, we will raise a notion of disciplinary knowledge from Vygotsky’s (1986) theory of concept formation. From this notion, we raise a proposal of argumentation as a process of generalization of tensions, which allows us to reconstruct the relational nature of disciplinary knowledge.
Argumentative language and conceptual development: Argumentation as a process of generalization
Language and theory of concepts in Vygotsky
For Vygotsky (1986), every concept is a generalization, that is, “operations in which a particular and unique event is treated as a class of events” (Larrain, 2017, p. 3). However, while every meaning of the word is a generalization, not every meaning of the word is a concept or, at least, not a developed concept. For Vygotsky (1986), concepts, as forms of generalization, are developed; that is, concepts and words do not coincide at all because a word can imply very different ways of conceptualizing. The concepts are not given by the processes of maturation of the human being or emerge only by the action of this on the objects. It is the communicative social environment, pre-existing to the individual, which demands the use of language and, with it, the development of those concepts. Thus, concepts are not categories for labeling or classifying meanings represented in the mind. Rather they are processes of generalization that occur moment by moment in the use of language and from communicative social interaction.
The fact that the concept is developed in and by the means of social interaction does not mean that it is knowledge that is “outside” and that it is “transferred” directly by other people. On the contrary, once the individual discovers the word already created and functional in the socio-historical environment, they only begin to create the forms of generalization that these forms of language suppose, and they do so through its use in different communicative contexts and given the needs of social activity. Thus, two people may even use the same words, but that does not imply that they think generally in the same way.
In Chapter 5 of “Thought and Language,” Vygotsky outlines a complex and lengthy developmental sequence for concept formation. This process begins with children’s initial utterances, which are syncretic generalizations marked by a heavy reliance on perception and considerable instability, making them unconventional. Over time, this progresses towards more conventional generalizations, where abstraction and generalization merge. In Chapter 6 of “Thought and Language,” Vygotsky distinguishes between two types of concepts: everyday or complex concepts and scientific concepts. Everyday concepts, which are initially unstable and encompass diverse generalization methods ranging from highly perceptual to more stable and pragmatic, are acquired through unsystematic interactions. These interactions are crucial as they often reference the specific context of the child’s activities, facilitating the learning process. In this way, everyday concepts are rich in terms of concrete experiences, are a-conscious, and are not related to each other.
From instruction, that is, from a type of systematic, repetitive, and deliberate social interaction, a qualitatively different form of generalization arises, that is, the emergence of scientific concepts or, in the words of Vygotsky (1986), “true concepts.” These concepts are characterized by referring to other words and not to the direct context of children, which provides them with greater stability and conventionality. The use of a certain way of speaking and thinking typical of instructional activities (definitions, learning of written language, learning of second language, etc.) promotes the development of a structure of thought, which allows the creation of generalizations with a high verbal component.
Thus, the emergence of scientific concepts transforms the way in which generalization has occurred so far with everyday concepts. This transformation is evident in the emergence of a system of generalizations. The author illustrates the process of generalization following the following geographical metaphor:
“If we imagine the totality of concepts as distributed over the surface of a globe, the location of every concept may be defined by means of a system of coordinates, corresponding to longitude and latitude in geography. One of these coordinates will indicate the location of a concept between the extremes of maximally generalized abstract conceptualization and the immediate sensory grasp of an object (…). The second coordinate will represent the objective reference of the concept, the locus within reality to which it applies. (...) The “coordinates” of the concept determine all relations of the given concept to other, i.e., to its coordinate, superordinate, and subordinate concepts”. (pp. 199-200)
In this sense, scientific concepts or “true concepts” involve not only different levels of depth and hierarchical relationships (length), as a generalization, but also relations of communality (latitude), that is, of a diversity of possible paths that unfold in the language in the reconstruction of the concept. In the words of Larrain et al. (2019): “Scientific concepts can be thought of as occurring through multiple equivalent conceptual paths emerging only when speakers have at their disposition more than one way to equivalently think of a word (a semantic system)” (p. 458). These scientific concepts are relevant precisely because they suppose the individual formation of conceptual systems, which occur through the use of specific functional ways of using language (Guzmán et al., 2022).
For this very reason, it is difficult for students (at all educational levels) to develop scientific concepts. It is not enough to teach a concept for another to learn it since what could happen is that students learn the word but not necessarily the concept. Therefore, although the word spoken for communicative purposes is not the concept, its use in social interaction is (pre)formative of the concept, while the concept is developed by trying different ways of generalizing the word of signifying.
Verbal thinking is a constructive process of generalization and of generalization of generalizations of various ways of producing. Since the process of generalization involves the construction of a system in the functional use of the word, the reconstruction of this system is not static, but is a movement of thought. Therefore, conceptualizing consists of building and reconstructing a system and positioning the concepts in a certain mode of relationship.
The dialectical nature of concepts
Vygotsky (1986) argues that scientific concepts are defined by a structured system of meanings that enable systematic, stable, and conventional generalizations. This system goes beyond mere dictionary definitions, representing a specific, systematic interrelation of words manifested in particular linguistic uses. These relationships include similarity, contradiction, and hierarchy, among other systematic word relations or methods of generalization. Consequently, a fully developed concept embodies a relational perspective and a cognitive trajectory that constantly anticipates alternatives, as outlined by the underlying system (Larrain & Haye, 2014).
Moreover, every structure or system is built on the basis of oppositions and contradictions that draw the basic relational field. This is how we read Vygotsky’s theory of concepts (1986): a concept, as a systematic way of thinking and generalizing, consists of contradictions that have a formative role in the system of meanings or words that act as foundation. In fact, if we were to prohibit contradictions, it would be impossible to imagine the system to which Vygotsky (1986) refers.
The dialectical nature of every concept is not a new proposal. Blunden (2012) already noted that every scientific concept is contradictory in itself. Moreover, the history of the production of scientific and disciplinary knowledge allows us to better understand this dialectical nature of concepts. Scientifically accepted ideas, in general, respond to alternatives that have been shown to be less effective solutions to answer the questions and problems that create them from a social, scientific, and political point of view. Thus, each historically produced concept, which is systematically linked to other concepts, does so in relation to refutation, approval, complementation, etc.
Concepts are not neutral and are directed at and stressed with other possible concepts. In the words of Bakhtin (1981) each word unfolds in a tense field of words from which it establishes all kinds of dialogical relations. Moreover, Billig (1996) raises a rhetorical notion of meaning that implies that each unit of thought and meaning has the structure of an argument that rises in the face of its opposing or counterargument. We may say that the contradiction is one of these structuring dialogical relations of both scientific activity and the structure of knowledge of disciplines.
It follows that if university students must reconstruct concepts as units of disciplinary and professional knowledge, they need to reconstruct not a dictionary definition, but an entire relational structure organized by strained relations of opposition and contradiction and, probably, with an argumentative structure (see Billig, 1997). How to achieve this? Our proposal is that argumentation, as a discursive practice, has a particularly important potential to promote the reconstruction of the dialectical and argumentative nature of every concept.
Argumentation as a process of generalization of contradictions.
Vygotsky (1986) emphasizes that the essence of concept development lies in the functional application of language. This perspective suggests that language, in its dynamic and varied utilization across social contexts, rather than as a static system, furnishes the means for conceptualization. Certain distinctive linguistic practices are instrumental in leading the way for the emergence of scientific concepts. In fact, the focus on language as a group of heterogeneous activities is shared by Wittgenstein (1953). Bakhtin’s (1986) notion of discourse genres would also point to the diversity of forms of language according to the diversity of social activities, even more so than typical forms of social activity. Taking this into account, it is crucial to note that it is this diversity that is key to the formative power of language (Bertau, 2011).
In this framework, we understand argumentation as a particularly powerful social and language practice to promote the reconstruction of the dialectical structure of scientific concepts, while promoting a particular form of generalization: the generalization of relationships, including especially those of opposition and contradiction. While Vygotsky (1986) does not notice its particular role in concept formation, Piaget (2001) does recognize its unique role in the reconstruction of knowledge structures, especially when it happens between peers.
Arguing, as a functional use of language and as a verbal thought process, has highly specific characteristics of its own. According to Leitão (2000), argumentation has a potential for the construction of knowledge that is based on its semiotic mechanisms that move following a dialectical structure: argument, counterargument, and response. In the argumentation, a justified position must be constructed and raised, and any justification assumes the possibility of the emergence of counterarguments. Then, at the time of the counterargument, the individual has the possibility of reviewing their own position; for the author, this would account for a meta-cognitive process that favors learning. In this regard: The discursive actions of justifying positions and reacting to contrary positions reorient the individual’s focus of attention from the phenomena of the world (on which they argue) towards their own statements (points of view, knowledge) about such phenomena [Las acciones discursivas de justificar posiciones y reaccionar a posiciones contrarias reorientan el foco de atención del individuo de los fenómenos del mundo (sobre los que argumenta) hacia sus propias afirmaciones (puntos de vista, conocimiento) sobre tales fenómenos] (Leitão, 2007, p. 16). Finally, we reach the answer, which is a critical process that may or may not integrate dissent between opposing positions.
Although Leitão (2000) identifies the epistemic potential of argumentation focused on its potential for revision of thought, since thought bends over itself to be revised, we emphasize another aspect so far less addressed: its potential for re-construction of contradictions. With the argumentation, then, the dialectical structure of the different concepts involved is generalized, tending their formation and re-construction at the individual level. It is not a question of forming a representation, but of reconstructing relationships between meanings of words in tension and contradiction, enriching a horizon of meaning that at first is obscure or opaque.
The formative character of argumentation has been recognized by other authors as an essential part of its epistemic dimension. For example, de Vries et al. (2002) emphasize the role of argumentation in conceptual diversification. Larrain (2017) and Larrain et al. (2019), based on the theory of concepts of Vygotsky (1986), propose that the dialectical movements of argumentation promote the imagination and figuration of the complex and contradictory field of meanings that characterizes every concept. Our proposal embarks on this line of thought, emphasizing the potential of the construction of concepts through the use of argumentation. Since its constituent movements (argument and counterargument) would allow the reconstruction of the contradictory and relational nature of scientific concepts through the progressive generalization of this difference. These discursive movements involve the possibility of constructing and reconstructing the process of generalization, trying new ways of generalizing the concepts put in dispute.
Argumentation, as a social and discursive practice, and as a linguistic activity that contributes to the development and refinement of disciplinary and professional concepts, extends beyond interactions between two or more individuals. Following Leitão (2000) and Billig (1996), we recognize the rhetorical and dialogical nature of argumentation as a type of language that, although used by a single speaker, contains different positions and relations of contradiction and response. Thus, we conceive of argumentation as a type of language that can be appropriated, developing what we call individual argumentation skills, and internalized, which is transformed and used not to influence others but ourselves (Kuhn, 2018).
From the previous reflection we can claim that, if internalized, argumentative language can serve to reconstruct concepts even when it is not argued with other people, but an argumentative dynamic is developed on an individual level. Moreover, following recent results (Larrain et al., 2020), it is possible to think that the epistemic dimension of argumentation always involves, at least in part, the use of internalized language promoting the formation of concepts through it.
Discussion
The central objective of this study has been to raise a vision of argumentative practice emphasizing its epistemic character. The relationship between argumentation and the construction of knowledge is long-standing. However, the notion of argumentative practice as a pedagogical means of knowledge construction has had more impact on school education than on university education. Moreover, at the university level, there remains a way of conceiving argumentation as a cognitive linguistic skill that depends on the previous acquisition of knowledge and not as a practice of knowledge construction.
In this paper we have raised the notion of argumentation, from a Vygotskian perspective, as a process of generalization – and therefore as a productive process of creating a system – of contradictions and tensions, which characterize scientific concepts. For this reason, we sustain that argumentative practice is a valuable resource, which, in itself, tends to the construction of scientific knowledge, especially when this discursive practice has been internalized.
We believe that emphasizing the epistemic character of argumentation is particularly relevant in teaching in higher education, for various reasons. On one hand, because of the difficulty it poses for students, especially in their early years, to reconstruct, visualize and imagine a whole history of development of scientific knowledge regarding the discipline, as well as entering a new community with a highly specialized discourse. Blunden (2012) points out that concepts are historical and cultural products that involve interactions in language and social life forms. In this new communicative social context, the student will discover new words, new discourse(s), new concepts of each science and scientific discipline, or new ways of generalizing concepts already known.
On the other hand, we acknowledged that argumentative ability is crucial for professional practice and collaborative living. However, there is a concern regarding the proficiency of this skill among university students. Research indicates that argumentative practice not only contributes to knowledge construction and learning enhancement but also to the cultivation of argumentative skills (Vogel et al., 2017). Moreover, under our notion of argumentation, we believe that it is important to develop argumentative skills through argumentative practice, given the ethical dimension of not doing so.
From the perspective we have adopted in this discourse, several implications emerge. Initially, it is imperative to explore the role of argumentative practice within the university teaching context. This exploration is pertinent given the preliminary evidence indicating a lack of discussion and debate, which are anticipated components of university education (Archila et al., 2020). This evidence aligns with the broader challenges faced by higher education in fostering an active learning environment where students engage in dialogue, discussion, and deliberation throughout their educational journey.
Another relevant aspect for which it would be useful to incorporate argumentation in teaching is the importance of the diversity of discursive uses and practices, which seems to be central to the development of thought (Bertau, 2011). This also means that incorporating argumentation as a pedagogical practice does not mean that all teaching throughout the curriculum is argumentative, but rather it involves students in a diversity of discursive practices. In this regard, and in the face of the practical and political challenges posed by the systematic and explicit incorporation of argumentation, it seems necessary to investigate empirically in what curricular space, moment, and in what way it would be more pertinent and effective to provide students with this resource to build knowledge.
One way to incorporate this type of dialogue is by systematically promoting spaces for discussion between peers, using methodologies such as university debates, problem-based learning, or dialogic argumentation practices in small groups or a whole class. For example, Argument-Driven Inquiry (ADI) is a recognized structured methodology with clear steps for teaching and learning science. Although the incorporation of these methodologies must be adapted to contexts.
Some clarifications related to the theory of concepts and argumentation understood from that place also bring implications for university education. In the university context, it is possible that the use of the word, for example, in evaluative contexts, suggests a deeper and more complex form of conceptualization than the student has done. This is likely considering certain forms of evaluation; for example, as Blunden (2012) points out, the task of characterizing a thing or listing everything we “know” about something generates a pseudo-concept rather than a true concept. Conceptualization, as a process that develops ontogenetically, cannot simply be transmitted by an adult or university professor. Recall that the central thesis of Vygotsky (1986) is that concepts are developed, which means that the verbal thought process in which a generalization system emerges is a flexible and dynamic process as they are not static entities (Larrain & Haye, 2014).
In this paper, we have delineated a methodology to elucidate the processes through which flexible generalizations evolve and to examine the progression towards novel forms of generalization. In this regard, we believe that both conceptual development and argumentative practice are closely related processes. This is because all these movements that occur in argumentative language involve an organization of the specific generalization system, an organization of relations of generality in which the contradiction of the concepts is incorporated. In this sense, it is highly difficult to reconstruct and form concepts if students are not made to participate in certain ways of speaking, such as argumentation, that is. It is not a question of knowing about argumentation but of participating in this discursive practice in order to know how to argue and think disciplinarily.
If argumentation is understood in this way, it involves, at the same time, a way of thinking and using language. In this sense, although the process of generalization has been conceptualized as a process that occurs moment by moment, there are certain common ways of generalizing which are the result of educational instruction, the history of concepts, and the similar social situations in which we participate. In the university classroom, we want students to be able to reconstruct these conventional scientific generalization systems. However, arguing also makes it possible to question these common systems of generalization.
We have offered an interpretation of argumentation from the theory of concepts of Vygotsky (1986) that accounts for argumentative discourse as a process of generalization that occurs in communicative social interaction and that is formative in nature in thought and in the generation of knowledge. Evidencing, with it, the discursive, dialogical, and dialectical character of argumentative language and the construction of scientific knowledge. The perspective that we have conceptualized is in alignment with the proposal of Leitão (2000) as evidence of how, in the use of language corresponding to the discursive movements in argumentation, the student constructs a certain type of conceptual knowledge.
The incorporation of the theory of concepts of Vygotsky (1986) allows us to complicate and expand the notion of the construction of knowledge with that specific emphasis. Although we do not emphasize the metacognitive character of the practice as the author does, this is compatible as long as the systems of generalization are repeated and acquire a conventional character. Furthermore, both positions highlight the dialectical dimension of argumentative practice and, in our case, also of scientific concepts. We believe that it is relevant to continue theorizing in the epistemic nature of argumentation, in order to make sense of the available empirical evidence that relieves the epistemic dimension of this practice.
Final declarations
Authors’ Contribution. All authors have contributed equally to the construction of the article and agree with its content.
Conflicts of Interest. The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Funding. This reflective-theoretical paper is not derived from any funded research project.
Ethical implications. There is no financial interest to report, and there are no ethical implications.
Andrews, R. (2009). Argumentation in Higher Education: improving practice through theory and research. 1st Ed. Routledge. ISBN 13: 978-1-135-27651-5 ePub
Andrews, R., Bilbro, R., Mitchell, S., Peake, K., Prior, P., Robinson, A., Huat, B., Torgerson, C. (2006). Argumentative skills in first year undergraduates: A pilot study. Higher Education Academy. https://www.advance-he.ac.uk/knowledge-hub/argumentative-skills-first-year-undergraduates-pilot-study
Antonio, R. P., & Prudente, M. S. (2021). Metacognitive Argument-Driven Inquiry in Teaching Antimicrobial Resistance: Effects on Students Conceptual Understanding and Argumentation Skills. Turkish Journal of Science Education, 2. https://doi.org/10.36681/tused.2021.60
Archila, P. A., Molina, J., & Truscott de Mejía, A.M. (2020). Using Historical Scientific Controversies to Promote Undergraduates’ Argumentation. Science & Education, 29 (3), 647-671. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-020-00126-6
Asterhan, C., & Schwarz, B. (2016). Argumentation for learning: Well-trodden paths and unexplored territories. Educational Psychologist, 51 (2), 164-187. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2016.1155458
Bakhtin, M. (1981). The dialogical imagination. 1st Ed. University of Texas Press. ISBN 0-292-71527-7
Bertau, M. (2011). Language for the Other: Constructing Cultural- Historical Psycholinguistics. Tätigkeitstheorie: E-Journal for Activity Theoretical Research in Germany, (5), 13-49.
Billig, M. (1996). Arguing and thinking: A rhetorical approach to social psychology. 2nd Ed. Cambridge University Press. ISBN: 9780521567398
Blunden, A. (2012). Concepts: A critical approach. 1st Ed. Brill. ISBN: 978-90-04-22847-4
Børte, K., Nesje, K., & Lillejord, S. (2020). Barriers to student active learning in higher education, Teaching in Higher Education, 1–19. doi:10.1080/13562517.2020.1839746
Buitrago, Á., Mejía Cuencua, N. M. M., & Hernández, R. H. (2013). La argumentación: De la retórica a la enseñanza de las ciencias. Innovación Educativa, 13 (29) . https://www.scielo.org.mx/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1665-26732013000300003
Bulgren, J. A., Ellis, J. D., & Marquis, J. G. (2014). The use and effectiveness of an argumentation and evaluation intervention in science classes. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 23(1), 82-97. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-013-9452-x
Daniels, J. (2017). Professional learning in higher education: Making good practice relevant. International Journal for Academic Development, 22 (2), 170-181. https://doi.org/10.1080/1360144X.2016.1261352
Daumiller, M., Rinas, R., Olden, D., & Dresel, M. (2021). Academics’ motivations in professional training courses: Effects on learning engagement and learning gains. International Journal for Academic Development, 26 (1), 7-23. https://doi.org/10.1080/1360144X.2020.1768396
de Vries, E., Lund, K., & Baker, M. (2002). Computer-mediated epistemic dialogue: Explanation and argumentation as vehicles for understanding scientific notions. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 11, 63-103.
Domínguez, M., & Conforti, C. M. (2019). ¿Por qué Lógica y Teoría de la Argumentación?, Andamios Revista de Investigación Social,16 (41), 163-173. https://doi.org/10.29092/uacm.v16i41.720
Driver, R., Newton, P., & Osborne, J. (2000). Establishing the norms of scientific argumentation in classrooms. Science Education, 84 (3), 287.
Fan, Y., Wang, T., & Wang, K. (2020). Studying the effectiveness of an online argumentation model for improving undergraduate students’ argumentation ability. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning. 36 (4), 1-14.
Fraser, K., Ryan, Y., Bolt, S., Copeman, P., Cottman, C., Fisher, M. B., Fleming, J., & Luzeckyj, A. (2019). Contemporary induction to teaching in Australian universities. International Journal for Academic Development, 24 (3), 286-300. https://doi.org/10.1080/1360144X.2019.1612751
Gutiérrez, G., & Carrasco, A. (2021). Chile’s enduring educational segregation: A trend unchanged by different cycles of reform. British Educational Research Journal, 47 (6), 1611-1634.
Guzmán, V.F., González-Palta, I., Larrain, A.S. (2022). Concept Formation. In: Glăveanu, V.P. (eds) The Palgrave Encyclopedia of the Possible. Palgrave Macmillan.
Hasnunidah, N., Susilo, H., Irawati, M., & Suwono, H. (2020). The contribution of argumentation and critical thinking skills on students’ concept understanding in different learning models. Journal of University Teaching & Learning Practice, 17 (1), https://doi.org/10.14453/jutlp.v17i1.6
Iordanou, K., Kuhn, D., Matos, F., Shi, Y., & Hemberger, L. (2019). Learning by arguing. Learning and Instruction, 63, 101207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.05.004
Ives, J., & Castillo-Montoya, M. (2020). First-Generation College Students as Academic Learners: A Systematic Review. Review of Educational Research, 90 (2), 139-178. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654319899707
Jiménez-Aleixandre, M. P. & Erduran, S. (2008). Argumentation in science education: An overview. In S. Erduran & M. P. Jiménez Aleixandre (Eds.), Argumentation in science education: Perspectives from classroom-based research. Springer.
Kuhn, D. (2010). Teaching and learning science as argument. Science Education, 94 (5), 810-824. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20395
Kuhn, D. (2018). A Role for Reasoning in a Dialogic Approach to Critical Thinking. Topoi, 37, 121-128. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-016-9373-4
Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. University of Chicago Press
Larrain, A. (2017). Argumentation and concept development: The role of imagination. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 32 (4), 521-536. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-016-0316-7
Larrain, A., & Burrows, F. (2020). Las pautas sí importan. Efecto del uso de pautas sobre la calidad de la escritura argumentativa en la universidad. Formación universitaria, 13(1), 115-126. https://doi.org/10.4067/S0718-50062020000100115
Larrain, A., & Haye, A. (2014). A dialogical conception of concepts. Theory & Psychology, 24(4), 459-478. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354314538546
Larrain, A., Brasi, L. D., Calderón, M., & Calzetta, A. (2021). Creencias docentes acerca de la enseñanza de la argumentación en el ciclo básico de formación. Formación universitaria, 14 (1), 99-110. https://doi.org/10.4067/S0718-50062021000100099
Larrain, A., Freire, P., López, P., & Grau, V. (2019). Counter-Arguing During Curriculum-Supported Peer Interaction Facilitates Middle-School Students’ Science Content Knowledge. Cognition and Instruction, 37 (4), 453-482. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2019.1627360
Larrain, A., Freire, P., Moretti, R., Requena, M., & Sabat, B. (2015). ¿La universidad en Chile promueve las habilidades de argumentación escrita? Un estudio exploratorio comparativo de estudiantes de educación universitaria y educación técnica. Calidad en la educación, (43), 201-228.
Larrain, A., Singer, V., Strasser, K., Howe, C., López, P., Pinochet, J., Moran, C., Sánchez, Á., Silva, M., & Villavicencio, C. (2020). Argumentation skills mediate the effect of peer argumentation on content knowledge in middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 113 (4), 736–753. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000619
Leitão, S. (2000). The Potential of Argument in Knowledge Building. Human Development, 43 (6), 332-360. https://doi.org/10.1159/000022695
Leitão, S. (2007). La dimensión epistémica de la argumentación. En E. Kronmüller y C. Cornejo (eds) Ciencias de la mente: Aproximaciones desde Latinoamérica, JC Sáez Editor. ISBN: 978-956-306-034-8
Mok, K. H., & Marginson, S. (2021). Massification, diversification and internationalisation of higher education in China: Critical reflections of developments in the last two decades. International Journal of Educational Development, 84, 102405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijedudev.2021.102405
Mulyati, Y., & Hadianto, D. (2023). Enhancing Argumentative Writing Via Online Peer Feedback-Based Essay: A Quasi-Experiment Study. International Journal of Instruction, 16(2), 195-212. https://doi.org/10.29333/iji.2023.16212a
Ndebele, Clever. (2022). Examining the efficacy of professionalizing university teaching through formal teaching qualifications at a historically disadvantaged university in South Africa. The Independent Journal of Teaching and Learning, 17 (1), 23-38.
Nnanyereugo, P., & Bolaji M. (2020). Effects of Dialogical Argumentation Instructional Model on Pre-service Teachers’ Ability to Solve Conceptual Mathematical Problems in Physics, African Journal of Research in Mathematics, Science and Technology Education, 24 (1), 129-141, doi: 10.1080/18117295.2020.1748325
Osborne, J. (2010). Arguing to learn in science: The role of collaborative, critical discourse. Science, 328, 463-466. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1183944
Pabuccu, A., & Erduran, S. (2017). Beyond rote learning in organic chemistry: The infusion and impact of argumentation in tertiary education. International Journal of Science Education, 39 (9), 1154-1172. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2017.1319988
Piaget, J. (2001). The language and thought of the child. 1st Ed. Routledge. ISBN 9780415267502.
Quintana, R., & Correnti, R. (2019). The right to argue: Teaching and assessing everyday argumentation skills. Journal of Further and Higher Education, 43(8), 1133-1151. https://doi.org/10.1080/0309877X.2018.1450967
Schwarz, B. (2009). Argumentation and learning. In N. Muller Mirza & A. Perret-Clermont (Eds.), Argumentation and education. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-98125-3
Si, J., Kong, H.-H., & Lee, S.-H. (2019). Developing Clinical Reasoning Skills Through Argumentation with the Concept Map Method in Medical Problem-Based Learning. Interdisciplinary Journal of Problem-Based Learning, 13 (1). https://doi.org/10.7771/1541-5015.1776
Sönmez, E., Kabataş Memiş, E. & Yerlikaya, Z. (2021). The effect of practices based on argumentation-based inquiry approach on teacher candidates’ critical thinking, Educational Studies, 47 (1), 59-83, doi: 10.1080/03055698.2019.1654364
Tiruneh, D. T., Verburgh, A., & Elen, J. (2014). Effectiveness of Critical Thinking Instruction in Higher Education: A Systematic Review of Intervention Studies. Higher Education Studies, 4 (1), 1-17.
Vogel, F., Wecker, C., Kollar, I., & Fischer, F. (2017). Socio-Cognitive Scaffolding with Computer-Supported Collaboration Scripts: A Meta-Analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 29(3), 477-511. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-016-9361-7
Vandelannote, I., & Demanet, J. (2021). Unravelling socioeconomic school composition effects on higher education enrollment: The role of students’ individual and shared feelings of futility and self-efficacy. Social Psychology of Education: An International Journal, 24(1), 169–193. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-021-09608-z
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and Language. The MIT Press. ISBN 13 978-0-262-22029-3
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Investigaciones filosóficas. 3rd Ed. Crítica. ISBN 84-7423-343-7